The science of tickling: why the brain won’t let us tickle ourselves (2024)

The first thing to understand about our inability to self-tickle is that it’s just one example of a widespread phenomenon: humans respond differently to touch depending on whether the sensation was created by ourselves or something else.

If you clap your hands, then have someone else clap one of your hands with theirs, you will generally perceive the latter as more intense. This difference in how we perceive ourselves and other things in the environment isn’t limited to humans, or to touch. In 2003, a study showed that crickets perceive their own chirps as quieter than those of other crickets.

Having this ability makes sense in evolutionary terms, says Dr Konstantina Kilteni at the Karolinska Institute in Stockholm, Sweden. It’s useful to know if a sensation is worth paying attention to or not. ‘If you have a bug crawling up your arm, you want to be sure you notice that,’ she said.

Body ownership

A prerequisite to this is that our brains have a sense of body ownership, so that we know whether a touch comes from our own moving fingers, say, or some foreign object. Understanding how this works is probably a crucial part of getting to grips with tickling. Dr Kilteni says that a raft of studies began to probe this in the late 1990s, but while they established a link between the intensity of touch and where it originates, they didn’t explore the precise conditions for this. She began the Tickle Me project in 2017 to go deeper.

One of her key experiments involved looking at the way people perceived touches on their fingers using a clever set up of levers. In the first part of the experiment, people touched a lever with their left forefinger, which instantly triggered a second lever to touch their right forefinger.

Dr Kilteni then compared this with two variations. In the first, people let their left finger rest on a plate above the first lever, then the plate was removed letting the finger fall onto the lever. This triggered the second lever to touch the right finger, but crucially this was now involuntary. In a final variation, the right finger was touched by the lever without any input from the person at all. It turned out that people perceived the touches generated by these three methods as successively more intense, even though they were all made with the same force. This suggests that if the brain knows a touch is coming, it feels it as less intense. This confirms that one of the reasons we cannot tickle ourselves is because our brain has already planned it, says Dr Kilteni.

In a separate experiment that used the same lever equipment, Dr Kilteni also introduced a sneaky twist so that when the participants touched the first lever with one finger, there was a delay of a fraction of a second before the second lever touched their other finger. It turned out that this element of surprise was important; the delay made the sensation more intense. All this gives us another hint as to why self-tickling is so hard: when you tickle yourself it is hard to be caught unaware.

‘You have to get kind of rough with the rats to get them to laugh; it’s rough play they like.’

Dr Kilteni conducted a raft of experiments like this during her project, but perhaps the most telling paper she has produced came out just a few months ago and concerns an area of the brain called the somatosensory cortex, a part of the brain that receives sensory information from the body.

In one experiment she had 30 volunteers touch their index fingers together, and then separately have their fingers touched by a robot, while she scanned their brains using an fMRI machine. Some people seemed to perceive the self-touch as less intense than others, and Dr Kilteni could see that these individuals tended to have stronger connections between the somatosensory cortex and another area of the brain called the cerebellum.

Little brain

The cerebellum, or ‘little brain’, is found at the nape of the neck. It is central to the control of our bodies’ movements but it is also thought to play a crucial role overseeing cognitive processing. Think of the brain like a factory with different parts processing different information and the cerebellum is the quality control supervisor. Neuroscientists suspect that the cerebellum sends signals to dial down the perception of tickling in the somatosensory cortex when it is our own fingers, not someone else’s, at work. Dr Kilteni’s fMRI studies lend weight to that hypothesis.

Over in New Jersey, US, DrMarlies Oostland is planning to further probe this connection through her NeuroTick project. One of Dr Oostland’s project supervisors, Professor Michael Brecht, at the Bernstein Center for Computational Neuroscience at Humboldt University of Berlin, Germany, was the scientist who along with his colleague Dr Shimpei Ishiyama, discovered that rats are ticklish in 2016. They showed that when tickled, rats emit ultrasonic ‘laughs’ and that their somatosensory cortex lights up like a Christmas tree at the same time.

Tickling the rats didn’t come entirely naturally to Oostland when she had a go on a visit to Berlin. ‘I’m used to working with mice, so I was too gentle,’ she said. ‘You have to get kind of rough with the rats to get them to laugh; it’s rough play they like.’

Dr Oostland is beginning her project at Princeton University by making fundamental studies of how the cerebellum in mice predicts the animals’ movements. She is using probes to measure the activity of individual cells in the cerebellum of a mouse to understand what’s going on in its brain as she puffs air at their whiskers (which isn’t unpleasant but should be surprising).

Armed with this understanding, the plan is for her to then move to Prof. Brecht’s lab in Germany in two year’s time to study the connection between the cerebellum and somatosensory cortex and try to confirm whether and how the signals pass between the two.

As well as helping us build a better fundamental understanding of the most sophisticated object in the universe, the human brain, Dr Oostland says work like this could help us understand autism spectrum disorder better too. People who have an injury to the cerebellum soon after birth have a 36 times higher chance of developing autism later in life. We don’t fully understand why, but Dr Oostaland says fundamental studies like this could help.

The research in this article was funded by the EU. If you liked this article, please consider sharing it on social media.

The science of tickling: why the brain won’t let us tickle ourselves (2024)

FAQs

The science of tickling: why the brain won’t let us tickle ourselves? ›

Our studies at University College London have shown that the cerebellum can predict sensations when your own movement causes them but not when someone else does. When you try to tickle yourself, the cerebellum predicts the sensation and this prediction is used to cancel the response of other brain areas to the tickle.

Why are we not ticklish when we tickle ourselves? ›

When somebody else tickles you, your muscles haven't got a plan from your brain, so the feeling is surprising – and ticklish! But you can't tickle yourself, because your brain is always one step ahead, telling your muscles and senses what to expect and stopping you from giving yourself a surprise.

Why is it possible to tickle someone else but not to tickle yourself? ›

The reason you can't tickle yourself is that when you move a part of your own body, a part of your brain monitors the movement and anticipates the sensations that it will cause.

What does tickling do to the brain? ›

During a tickle, the skin's nerve endings shoot electrical signals to the somatosensory cortex, a part of the brain that processes touch. Meanwhile, the anterior cingulate cortex analyzes these signals as either harmful or playful. But in the back part of the brain, the cerebellum gives you away.

Why can't I handle being tickled? ›

People may hate being tickled due to the loss of control over their bodies, experts say. Tickling can overwhelm the nervous system, causing actual, if temporary, paralysis, Alan Fridlund, Ph. D., associate professor in the department of psychological and brain sciences at the University of California, told Vice.

How rare is it to be able to tickle yourself? ›

2% of the world can tickle themselves. That means that just because you can tickle yourself doesn't mean you're schizophrenic. According to research, schizophrenic people can tickle themselves, but not all people who can tickle themselves are schizophrenic.

Can you desensitize yourself to tickling? ›

You're less likely to feel ticklish if your brain can predict the movement before it happens. By putting your hand on the tickler's, their tickling motions will be more predictable to your brain, and you won't feel as ticklish when they touch you. Relax the muscles that are being tickled.

Why shouldn't you tickle babies' feet? ›

Tickling baby feet can simulate being held down, touched, or violated without consent, which can be highly triggering and traumatic for some children. Even if tickling baby feet is done with good intentions and no harm intended, it can still cause emotional distress and flashbacks.

Can non schizophrenic people tickle themselves? ›

When a neurotypical person without schizophrenia or schizotypy goes to tickle themselves, their brain recognizes that it's ordered the hand to stimulate the ticklish spot. The brain predicts the outcome (tickling), and reduces that sensation (though we're not really sure why that happens, either).

What percentage of humans are not ticklish? ›

While many people assume that other people enjoy tickling, a recent survey of 84 college students indicated that only 32% of respondents enjoy being tickled, with 32% giving neutral responses and 36% stating that they do not enjoy being tickled.

What is the most ticklish part of the human body? ›

Common ticklish spots

The most ticklish spots tend to be those that are also the most sensitive, like your: Feet. Neck. Ribcage.

Is tickling mental or physical? ›

In terms of neurology, the sensation of being tickled activates the hypothalamus, the area of the brain that regulates behavioral responses and controls metabolic processes. It also involves the cerebral cortex, which manages sensory information.

Why are feet ticklish but not hands? ›

One reason feet are ticklish is that they have more densely packed nerve receptors than most other places on the body. Upwards of 8,000 nerve endings can be found in your foot. This huge amount of nerves makes your feet more sensitive than other body parts, and more ticklish, too.

Why is it impossible to tickle yourself? ›

Pressure on certain sensitive parts of the body can lead to a tickling sensation. Yet even the most ticklish person will have a hard time trying to tickle themselves. That's because our brains anticipate our touch, effectively canceling out our own tickles.

Can too much tickling be bad? ›

“Excessive tickling can also lead to anxiety in children. Not only this, but tickling can even cause death from asphyxia, brain aneurysms or other stress-related injuries when done constantly. It is better to avoid tickling the baby to make sure that there are no problems due to it,” said Dr Kathwate.

What if you get tickled for too long? ›

It can become torture, if it's done long enough and while tickling itself won't kill you, the stress could trigger such things as a heart attack or stroke. And if it goes on too long, it becomes very painful.

Is it rare to not be ticklish? ›

Is everyone ticklish? No, not everyone is ticklish. The tickle response varies from person to person.

Where is the only place you can tickle yourself? ›

This doesn't always work, but some people report a mildly ticklish sensation when they barely touch their skin with their fingertips and move them around in a circular motion. The best places for this are the inside of your elbow, your neck, or the back of your knee.

What happens if you tickle yourself? ›

For the same reason, we can't tickle ourselves. If we grab our sides in an attempt to tickle ourselves, our brain anticipates this contact from the hands and prepares itself for it. By taking away the feeling of unease and panic, the body no longer responds the same as it would if someone else were to tickle us.

Why are some people not ticklish? ›

Variations in ticklishness from person to person can be attributed to several factors, including genetic differences, level of tickle exposure, and individual psychological traits. Our tickle response may be partially coded in our genes.

Top Articles
Tony Stark House: The Truth About the Real Life Iron Man House
MCU: Iron Man’s House Is Real & Here Is Where It’s Located
WALB Locker Room Report Week 5 2024
Tyler Sis 360 Louisiana Mo
Jennifer Hart Facebook
80 For Brady Showtimes Near Marcus Point Cinema
From Algeria to Uzbekistan-These Are the Top Baby Names Around the World
Top Financial Advisors in the U.S.
The Pope's Exorcist Showtimes Near Cinemark Hollywood Movies 20
Hallowed Sepulchre Instances & More
Mndot Road Closures
Best Private Elementary Schools In Virginia
Santa Clara Valley Medical Center Medical Records
Top Hat Trailer Wiring Diagram
Conduent Connect Feps Login
Hssn Broadcasts
Gas Station Drive Thru Car Wash Near Me
Cvb Location Code Lookup
Used Sawmill For Sale - Craigslist Near Tennessee
"Une héroïne" : les funérailles de Rebecca Cheptegei, athlète olympique immolée par son compagnon | TF1 INFO
Tvtv.us Duluth Mn
R Cwbt
St Maries Idaho Craigslist
Stardew Expanded Wiki
Nhl Tankathon Mock Draft
Walgreens Alma School And Dynamite
R&S Auto Lockridge Iowa
Craigslist Roseburg Oregon Free Stuff
Cookie Clicker Advanced Method Unblocked
Jeff Nippard Push Pull Program Pdf
Www Pointclickcare Cna Login
Cpt 90677 Reimbursem*nt 2023
Victory for Belron® company Carglass® Germany and ATU as European Court of Justice defends a fair and level playing field in the automotive aftermarket
Life Insurance Policies | New York Life
Sf Bay Area Craigslist Com
Barber Gym Quantico Hours
11301 Lakeline Blvd Parkline Plaza Ctr Ste 150
Lake Andes Buy Sell Trade
Vons Credit Union Routing Number
At Home Hourly Pay
Joey Gentile Lpsg
Arnesons Webcam
Arch Aplin Iii Felony
20 Mr. Miyagi Inspirational Quotes For Wisdom
Craigslist St Helens
Publix Store 840
Helpers Needed At Once Bug Fables
Morbid Ash And Annie Drew
Samantha Lyne Wikipedia
O.c Craigslist
BYU Football: Instant Observations From Blowout Win At Wyoming
Latest Posts
Article information

Author: Ouida Strosin DO

Last Updated:

Views: 5337

Rating: 4.6 / 5 (56 voted)

Reviews: 87% of readers found this page helpful

Author information

Name: Ouida Strosin DO

Birthday: 1995-04-27

Address: Suite 927 930 Kilback Radial, Candidaville, TN 87795

Phone: +8561498978366

Job: Legacy Manufacturing Specialist

Hobby: Singing, Mountain biking, Water sports, Water sports, Taxidermy, Polo, Pet

Introduction: My name is Ouida Strosin DO, I am a precious, combative, spotless, modern, spotless, beautiful, precious person who loves writing and wants to share my knowledge and understanding with you.